Author: Felix Schoeller

neural correlates of chills as shown by a frozen brain

The neural correlates of chills: How bodily sensations shape emotional experiences

E4001, Manuscript, Proceedings

Get The Article

  • Article PDF
  • Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience Article

Cite This Work

  • APA
  • MLA
  • Bibtex

Schoeller, F., Jain, A., Pizzagalli, D. A., & Reggente, N. (2024). The neurobiology of aesthetic chills: How bodily sensations shape emotional experiences. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-024-01168-x

Schoeller, Félix, Abhinandan Jain, et al. “The neurobiology of aesthetic chills: How bodily sensations shape emotional experiences.” Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, Feb. 2024, doi:10.3758/s13415-024-01168-x.

@article{Schoeller_Jain_Pizzagalli_Reggente_2024, title={The neurobiology of aesthetic chills: How bodily sensations shape emotional experiences}, url={https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-024-01168-x}, DOI={10.3758/s13415-024-01168-x}, journal={Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience}, author={Schoeller, Félix and Jain, Abhinandan and Pizzagalli, Diego A. and Reggente, Nicco}, year={2024}, month=feb }

Neural Correlates of Chills: How the Brain Creates a Powerful Emotional Response

Aesthetic chills are a universal emotional response characterized by shivers and goosebumps in reaction to specific rewarding or threatening stimuli, such as music, films, or speech. What makes this phenomenon so intriguing is that it simultaneously involves subjective feelings and measurable physical sensations, providing a tangible link between the mind and body.

The Role of Brain Regions and Networks

Recent research has shed light on the specific brain regions and networks involved in the experience of aesthetic chills. Understanding the neural correlates of chills helps us delve into fascinating questions about the mind-body connection.

Our review highlights key questions that aesthetic chills can help us answer: How precisely do bodily sensations influence emotional experiences? What is the role of prediction and uncertainty in shaping our feelings? And how does the brain balance processing rewards versus threats?

neural correlates of chills are vast and span the cerebrum, cerebellum, and brainstem

The Mesocorticolimbic System: A Key Player in Chills

By synthesizing evidence from neuroimaging studies, we propose that aesthetic chills engage a distinct brain network involving the mesocorticolimbic system. This network includes regions like the ventral tegmental area (VTA), nucleus accumbens (NAcc), amygdala (AMG), and frontal areas such as the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) and ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC). Crucially, the VTA releases dopamine, a neurotransmitter critical for reward processing and motivation, throughout these regions.

Chills, Reward, Learning, and the Brain’s Predictions

neural correlates of chills seem to depend on the learning rate

We suggest that aesthetic chills may correspond to peaks in consummatory pleasure, marking the transition from the “wanting” phase of reward to the “liking” and “learning” phases. This perspective aligns with the observation that chills often occur during the culmination of an aesthetic experience, such as the resolution of a narrative or musical tension.

neural correlates of chills seem associated with the anticipation and reward response.

Interoception and the Insula

The involvement of the insula, a region linked to interoception (the perception of internal bodily states), highlights the importance of peripheral signals in shaping the emotional quality of chills. This is further supported by findings that manipulating bodily sensations, such as enhancing the feeling of cold, can intensify the experience of chills and its downstream effects on cognition.

Individual Differences and the Experience of Chills

Interestingly, our susceptibility to aesthetic chills seems to be influenced by individual differences in personality traits like openness to experience and absorption, as well as biological factors such as gene variants affecting neurotransmitter function. This suggests that our propensity for chills is shaped by a complex interplay of psychological and neurobiological factors.

Dopamine, Prediction Errors, and Learning

We propose that the neurotransmitter dopamine plays a key role in aesthetic chills by encoding the precision of our brain’s predictions. When an aesthetic stimulus violates our expectations in a way that is ultimately rewarding, dopamine release signals the need to update our predictions, enhancing memory consolidation and learning. This process may underlie the heightened attention and memory effects observed during chills.

Mental Health Implications

Understanding the neurobiology of aesthetic chills has important implications for mental health. Dysfunctional precision encoding of prediction errors by dopamine is implicated in conditions like schizophrenia, depression, and addiction. Preliminary evidence suggests that experiencing aesthetic chills may help mitigate anhedonia (loss of pleasure) in depression by improving reward learning and shifting maladaptive self-beliefs. The therapeutic potential of chills lies in their ability to promote positive emotional states and cognitive flexibility.

Read more
this predicting chills image shows a row of people and one with frost on their glasses

Predicting Chills – Characterizing Individual Differences in Peak Emotional Response

E4001, Manuscript, Proceedings

Get The Article

Cite This Work

  • APA
  • MLA
  • Bibtex

Schoeller, F., Christov-Moore, L., Lynch, C., Diot, T., & Reggente, N. (2024). Predicting Individual Differences in Peak Emotional Response. PNAS Nexus, 3(3). https://doi.org/10.1093/pnasnexus/pgae066

Schoeller, Félix, Leonardo Christov-Moore, et al. “Predicting Individual Differences in Peak Emotional Response.” PNAS Nexus, vol. 3, no. 3, Feb. 2024, https://doi.org/10.1093/pnasnexus/pgae066.

@article{Schoeller_Christov-Moore_Lynch_Diot_Reggente_2024, title={Predicting Individual Differences in Peak Emotional Response}, volume={3}, url={https://doi.org/10.1093/pnasnexus/pgae066}, DOI={10.1093/pnasnexus/pgae066}, number={3}, journal={PNAS Nexus}, author={Schoeller, Félix and Christov-Moore, Leonardo and Lynch, Caitlin and Diot, Thomas and Reggente, Nicco}, year={2024}, month=feb }

Predicting Chills: Unraveling the Factors Behind a Powerful Emotional Response

Have you ever felt a shiver run down your spine when deeply moved by a piece of music or a scene in a film? Those “aesthetic chills” offer a fascinating glimpse into the interplay of our emotions and our individual experiences. In a recent study published in PNAS Nexus, we aimed to understand what makes some people more likely to feel these chills.

The Study Design

Our approach was multifaceted:

  • Stimuli Selection: We used innovative data mining techniques on social media platforms to curate a database of stimuli with a proven track record of inducing chills.
  • Diverse Participants: We exposed a diverse group of over 2,900 participants from Southern California to these stimuli. Data on their demographics, personality traits, and emotional responses were carefully collected.

Key Findings: Who’s Most Likely to Experience Chills

Our results were illuminating:

  • Demographics: Certain demographic factors, such as being middle-aged, highly educated, and male, were associated with a greater likelihood of experiencing chills.
  • Personality’s Impact: We also identified specific personality traits, like extraversion and conscientiousness, that were linked to more intense chills responses.
  • Microcultures and Resonance: Perhaps the most intriguing finding was the use of latent class analysis to uncover hidden “microcultures.” These subgroups, characterized by specific combinations of demographic and psychological attributes, were significantly more likely to experience chills. This points to the role of cultural resonance in shaping these emotional experiences.
predicting chills is hard - this image shows a bunch of people in a where's waldo style backdrop all looking at different pieces of content

Predictive Power: Can We Foresee Chills?

We pushed the analysis further by employing machine learning algorithms to see if we could predict the occurrence and intensity of chills based on a combination of personal characteristics. Our models achieved up to 73.5% accuracy in predicting whether someone would experience chills and accounted for 48% of the variance in chills intensity.

The Significance of Our Work

This study has far-reaching implications. By identifying the key factors that shape our susceptibility to aesthetic chills, we open doors to more targeted and personalized approaches to studying these experiences in a laboratory setting. Furthermore, understanding these “chills profiles” could pave the way for using music, art, or other stimuli in therapeutic contexts – perhaps helping reduce symptoms like anhedonia in depression.

Read more
individual differences in aesthetic chills

Individual Differences in Aesthetic Chills

E4001, Manuscript, Proceedings

Cite This Work

  • APA
  • MLA
  • Bibtex

Schoeller, F., Moore, L., Lynch, C., & Reggente, N. (2023c). ChillsDB 2.0: Individual Differences in aesthetic chills among 2,900+ Southern California participants. Scientific Data, 10(1). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41597-023-02816-6

Schoeller, Felix, et al. “ChillsDB 2.0: Individual Differences in Aesthetic Chills Among 2,900+ Southern California Participants.” Scientific data 10.1 (2023): 922.

@article{schoeller2023chillsdb, title={ChillsDB 2.0: Individual Differences in Aesthetic Chills Among 2,900+ Southern California Participants}, author={Schoeller, Felix and Christov Moore, Leo and Lynch, Caite and Reggente, Nicco}, journal={Scientific data}, volume={10}, number={1}, pages={922}, year={2023}, publisher={Nature Publishing Group UK London} }

Understanding Individual Differences in Aesthetic Chills

At IACS, we have been deeply engaged in the scientific exploration of aesthetic chills – those spine-tingling, goosebump-inducing responses evoked by stimuli such as music, films, and stories at large. These responses are recognized as a universal indicator of peak human experiences that transcend cultural boundaries.

Tools for Investigating Aesthetic Chills

One of our main goals is to build an open-source technological infrastructure for researchers to study chills in the lab. Our first output was ChillsDB, a database of audiovisual stimuli designed and validated to reliably induce aesthetic chills in a laboratory setting. This tool represented a breakthrough for the field, enabling researchers to investigate the psychological and neurological foundations of this intense emotional response under controlled conditions.

individual differences in aesthetic chills - this image shows a person viewing different pieces of content to emphasize how different people get chills from different content.

ChillsDB 2.0: Focusing on Individuality

We are now excited to announce the release of ChillsDB 2.0, published in Nature: Scientific Data, which marks a significant expansion of our initial efforts. In this updated version, we have enriched our dataset with inputs from nearly 3,000 diverse participants from Southern California. This enhancement not only includes responses to a selection of stimuli from our original database and new additions but also encompasses comprehensive data on participants’ demographics, personality traits, and emotional states before and after exposure to each stimulus.

The Therapeutic Potential of Aesthetic Chills

ChillsDB 2.0 has already proven to be a foundational resource for examining the therapeutic possibilities of aesthetic chills in treating conditions like depression. By elucidating the mechanisms behind these peak emotional states, we aim to discover novel methods for enhancing mood and introducing new perspectives to both clinical and general populations.

The Path Forward

While significant efforts are still required to comprehensively understand the phenomenology and neurobiology of aesthetic chills and to harness these insights for improving well-being, this new database represents an important step forward.

Read more
Aesthetic chills mitigate maladaptive cognition in depression-- an important finding showcasing the power of positive affect promotion.

Aesthetic chills mitigate maladaptive cognition in depression

E4001, Manuscript, Proceedings, Review

Get The Article

Cite This Work

  • APA
  • MLA
  • Bibtex

Schoeller, F., Jain, A., Adrien, V., Maes, P., & Reggente, N. (2024). Aesthetic chills mitigate maladaptive cognition in depression. BMC Psychiatry, 24(1). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-023-05476-3

“Aesthetic Chills Mitigate Maladaptive Cognition in Depression.” BMC Psychiatry, vol. 24, no. 1, Jan. 2024, https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-023-05476-3.

@article{Schoeller_Jain_Adrien_Maes_Reggente_2024c, title={Aesthetic chills mitigate maladaptive cognition in depression}, volume={24}, url={https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-023-05476-3}, DOI={10.1186/s12888-023-05476-3}, number={1}, journal={BMC Psychiatry}, author={Schoeller, Félix and Jain, Abhinandan and Adrien, Vladimir and Maes, Pattie and Reggente, Nicco}, year={2024}, month=jan }

Using peak positive affect (aesthetic chills) to help with depression

In our recent collaboration with Pattie Maes’s Fluid Interfaces group at MIT Media Lab and Dr. Vladimir Adrien from Assistance Publique Hôpitaux de Paris (APHP) in France, we investigated the potential for aesthetic chills to serve as an innovative intervention for major depressive disorder. This effort is a considerable advancement towards the notion of promoting positive affect in depression, which stands in contrast to standard care which is mostly focused on mitigating negative affect.

Instead of focusing on how to help individuals with depression not feel so bad, this work suggests the potential of helping those individuals by presenting them with content so they can feel good.

Aesthetic chills are characterized by sensations like shivers, goosebumps, and tingling that arise in response to emotional experiences with art, music, or nature. We hypothesized that by eliciting chills through validated multimedia stimuli, we could positively influence the core beliefs and self-schemas of individuals with depression. Across two studies with 96 participants diagnosed with major depressive disorder, we engaged participants in randomized sessions involving chill-inducing and neutral control stimuli across visual, auditory, and written modalities. Our results demonstrated that aesthetic chills induced a notable increase in self-acceptance among depressed participants. Chill-inducing stimuli appeared to facilitate positive emotional breakthroughs and shifts in self-perception that could address cognitive distortions related to depression. The data further suggest that aesthetic chills may engage reward-related neural pathways similarly to interventions like psychedelic-assisted therapy.

Individuals with major depressive disorder reported more emotional breakthroughs in their maladaptive cognition (e.g., lack of self-acceptance) when they reported getting chills compared to individuals who viewed the same content, but didn’t get chills. This also scaled as a function of the intensity of those chills.

While preliminary, these findings bring much-needed attention to the potential for aesthetic chills to positively influence core beliefs and schemas related to the self and one’s place in the world. For individuals with depression stemming from early adverse experiences, chill-inducing stimuli could foster emotional catharsis and lasting change to maladaptive self-narratives developed as coping mechanisms. Our research provides initial evidence that the biological processes involved in aesthetic chills can be harnessed for therapeutic ends. Chill-based interventions offer a promising avenue for large-scale study given the ease of dissemination through multimedia experiences.

Looking forward, further research should explore the neurophysiological mechanisms of aesthetic chills and biomarkers that may predict individual responses. Larger clinical trials are needed to investigate optimal protocols and delivery methods for chill-based therapy. We believe aesthetic chills represent an innovative non-pharmacological intervention that warrants greater attention from the psychiatry, psychology, and human-computer interaction communities.

shift + ⏎ to add a ne

Read more
interoceptive technologies for clinical use cases

Interoceptive Technologies for Psychiatric Interventions: A Comprehensive Review

E4001, Manuscript, Proceedings, Review

Cite This Work

  • APA
  • MLA
  • Bibtex

Schoeller, F., Horowitz, A. H., Jain, A., Maes, P., Reggente, N., Christov-Moore, L., . . . Friston, K. J. (2024). Interoceptive technologies for psychiatric interventions: From diagnosis to clinical applications. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 156, 105478. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2023.105478

Schoeller, Félix, Adam Haar Horowitz, et al. “Interoceptive Technologies for Psychiatric Interventions: From Diagnosis to Clinical Applications.” Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, vol. 156, Jan. 2024, p. 105478. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2023.105478.

@article{Schoeller_Horowitz_Jain_Maes_Reggente_Christov-Moore_Pezzulo_Barca_Allen_Salomon_et al._2024, title={Interoceptive technologies for psychiatric interventions: From diagnosis to clinical applications}, volume={156}, url={https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2023.105478}, DOI={10.1016/j.neubiorev.2023.105478}, journal={Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews}, author={Schoeller, Félix and Horowitz, Adam Haar and Jain, Abhinandan and Maes, Pattie and Reggente, Nicco and Christov-Moore, Leonardo and Pezzulo, Giovanni and Barca, Laura and Allen, Micah and Salomon, Roy and Miller, Mark and Di Lernia, Daniele and Riva, Giuseppe and Tsakiris, Manos and Chalah, Moussa A. and Klein, Arno and Zhang, Ben and Garcia, Teresa and Pollack, Ursula and Trousselard, Marion and Verdonk, Charles and Dumas, Guillaume and Adrien, Vladimir and Friston, Karl J.}, year={2024}, month=jan, pages={105478} }

What Is Interoception?

Interoception refers to our awareness of internal bodily signals like heartbeat, breathing, and digestion. While often overlooked, emerging research is revealing interoception as a fundamental process underlying emotion, cognition, and mental health. A new multidisciplinary review led by IACS senior research scientist Felix Schoeller and published in Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews explores the profound significance of interoception and its potential applications in psychiatric diagnosis and treatment.

Directly manipulating interoceptive signals in experiments has proven challenging due to the highly invasive techniques currently used, like esophageal balloon distension. There is also a lack of standardized, validated measures of interoceptive function across research disciplines as “the lack of correlation across unimodal tests underscores the need for multimodal approaches that assess integration of interoceptive information across bodily systems.” Drawing from fields like psychology, physiology, psychiatry, engineering, and neuroscience, the article provides a detailed account of the neurobiology of interoception, describing it as a hierarchical predictive processing system in the brain, and emphasizing the key role of dysfunctional interoceptive processing in disorders like anxiety, depression, and eating disorders.

What are Interoceptive Technologies?

The review also explores in details existing paradigms for modulating interoception, like interoceptive conditioning. This involves pairing internal bodily sensations with aversive stimuli to reshape emotional and physiological responses through a form of classical conditioning. The authors discuss clinical applications of these approaches, such as interoceptive exposure therapy for anxiety disorders. They also propose a new classification system for interoceptive technologies, dividing them into three categories: artificial sensations that induce novel bodily perceptions, interoceptive illusions that manipulate the precision of predictions, and emotional augmentation systems that facilitate beneficial changes in beliefs or behaviors.

interoceptive technologies examples

Figure 1. Overview of interoceptive technologies: A) the breath-holding test as an artificial sensation, whereby some bodily signal is directly manipulated, B) false heart feedback as an interoceptive illusion, where contextual cues generate a perceptual drift (here the illusion that the heart beats faster at a faster-than-expected rate), C) the therapeutic alliance as entrainment, where the patient’s heart rate slows down as the therapist’s is increasing, leading both to tend towards some average value, D) augmented exposure therapy as emotional augmentation, similar to B but with additional exteroceptive cues having personal significance to the individual (e.g. eliciting the trauma-related memory) favoring an emotional explanation for the interoceptive drift.

Such technologies could have powerful implications. Artificially inducing bodily sensations could help diagnose psychiatric conditions by testing patients’ susceptibility to developing skewed predictions about their internal state. More advanced emotional augmentation systems could precisely modulate predictive processes to reshape maladaptive cognitions and behaviors. While acknowledging that much remains unknown, the review shows the vast potential for interoceptive interventions to improve diagnosis and treatment of mental health disorders. Developing standardized measures and new technologies to precisely manipulate interoceptive signaling may open transformative frontiers in biological psychiatry and psychology.

Read more
picture of someone who has depression getting chills and then opening up some hope

Chills Foster Emotional Breakthrough In Depression

E4001, Proceedings
section-a8ead51

Get the Article

Aesthetic chills foster self-acceptance and emotional breakthrough in depression

Open Source Article

Cite This Work

Schoeller, Felix, et al. “Aesthetic Chills Foster Self-Acceptance and Emotional Breakthrough in Depression.” 2022, https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/rhftq.

Schoeller, F., Jain, A., Adrien, V., & Maes, P. (2022). Aesthetic chills foster self-acceptance and emotional breakthrough in depression. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/rhftq

Schoeller, Felix, Abhinandan Jain, Vladimir Adrien, and Pattie Maes. “Aesthetic Chills Foster Self-Acceptance and Emotional Breakthrough in Depression,” 2022. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/rhftq.

Schoeller F, Jain A, Adrien V, Maes P. Aesthetic chills foster self-acceptance and emotional breakthrough in depression. 2022 Dec 21;https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnins.2022.1013117/full

 

Schoeller, F., Jain, A., Adrien, V. and Maes, P. (2022). Aesthetic chills foster self-acceptance and emotional breakthrough in depression. doi:https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/rhftq.

section-ca3cc10

Chills Foster Emotional Breakthrough In Depression

Chills are a common emotional response to stimuli, whether it's from listening to your favorite music or engaging with deeply moving films. But did you know that this bodily response may hold potential for therapeutic intervention for individuals diagnosed with depression?

A recent exploratory study examined the effects of chills stimulation on subjects clinically diagnosed with depression. The study found that chill-inducing stimuli may have the potential to affect the core schema of depressed patients, specifically in terms of shame and self-acceptance. The results suggest that the mechanism of action during the chills response may resemble the form of problem resolution induced by the psychedelic and psychotherapeutic experience, leading to similar positive outcomes for the subject.

This study sheds light on the potential therapeutic value of aesthetic chills for reward-related or dopaminergic illnesses. Further research is needed to fully understand the effects of chills on mental health and to determine the feasibility and safety of using aesthetic chills as a therapeutic intervention.

It's exciting to think about the potential of aesthetic chills as a novel form of body-based experience to draw people out of anhedonia and depression and help them find meaning in life again. As research in this field progresses, we may see more developments in the use of chills stimulation as a therapeutic intervention for mental health.

section-0cc0b79
Abstract

Aesthetic chills, a strong emotional reaction characterized by a specific bodily response of thermoregulatory mechanisms such as shivers and goosebumps, may hold scientific and clinical potential for reward-related or dopaminergic illnesses. In this first exploratory study, we examined the effects of chills stimulation on subjects clinically diagnosed with depression. Our results suggest that chill-inducing stimuli may have the potential to affect the core schema of depressed patients, specifically in terms of shame and self-acceptance. These results suggest that the mechanism of action during the chills response may resemble the form of problem resolution induced by the psychedelic and psychotherapeutic experience, leading to similar positive outcomes for the subject. Further research is needed to fully understand the effects of chills on mental health and to determine the feasibility and safety of using aesthetic chills as a therapeutic intervention.

a graph showing how chills can have an impact on emotional breakthrough, which has huge implications for depression
Read more

IACS Research Featured in Vice

Press
section-979e9b4

Exciting news!

Our research has been featured in Vice magazine's motherboard section. Hannah Docter-Loeb from Motherboard wrote a short piece about our research on chills stimuli with MIT Media Lab and the Gonda Brain Research Centre.

Cite This Work

Vice. (2023, March 24). [web log]. Retrieved from https://www.vice.com/en/article/pkaywn/heres-a-database-of-media-scientifically-verified-to-give-you-the-chills.

Here's a Database of Media Scientifically Verified to Give You the Chills, Vice, 24 Mar. 2023, https://www.vice.com/en/article/pkaywn/heres-a-database-of-media-scientifically-verified-to-give-you-the-chills.

Web log. Here's a Database of Media Scientifically Verified to Give You the Chills (blog). Vice, March 24, 2023. https://www.vice.com/en/article/pkaywn/heres-a-database-of-media-scientifically-verified-to-give-you-the-chills.

Docter-Loeb, H. (2023) Here's a Database of Media Scientifically Verified to Give You the Chills. Vice, 24 March. Available at: https://www.vice.com/en/article/pkaywn/heres-a-database-of-media-scientifically-verified-to-give-you-the-chills.

Here’s a Database of Media Scientifically Verified to Give You the Chills [Internet]. www.vice.com. [cited 2023 Apr 12]. Available from: https://www.vice.com/en/article/pkaywn/heres-a-database-of-media-scientifically-verified-to-give-you-the-chills

 

Text Body

IACS Research Featured in Vice

The article explores the fascinating world of chills and the emotional response they elicit in individuals.

In an effort lead by Dr. Felix Schoeller at IACS, and in collaboration with the MIT Media Lab, and the Gonda Multidisciplinary Brain Research Centre, researchers have created a database of stimuli that have the potential to induce chills. The stimuli include music, film, and speech, and were compiled from social media platforms such as YouTube and Reddit.

We chose the top 50 videos from the database and randomly introduced them to over 600 participants on a crowdsourcing platform. The results showed that participants who experienced chills reported significantly more positive emotional valence and greater arousal during the experience, compared to those who did not experience chills.

We believe that understanding the emotional consequences of chills can help guide mental health interventions. They are exploring the option of using chills as a novel intervention for depression, and are even developing a device to artificially elicit the emotion and multiply its effects.

This is an exciting development in the field of mental health and could potentially offer a new avenue for treatment for individuals suffering from depression and other mood disorders. We are thrilled to be a part of this groundbreaking research and look forward to seeing what the future holds.

 

Read more

Why Do We Experience Chills?

Musings
section-ed2ea9b
Read the Article

Psychogenic Shivers: why we get the chills when we aren't cold 

Article

 

Cite This Work

Schoeller, F. (2021, April 8). Psychogenic shivers: Why we get the chills when we aren't cold: Aeon ideas. Psychogenic shivers: why we get the chills when we aren’t cold. Retrieved from https://aeon.co/ideas/psychogenic-shivers-why-we-get-the-chills-when-we-arent-cold

Schoeller, Felix. “Psychogenic Shivers: Why We Get the Chills When We Aren't Cold: Aeon Ideas.” Psychogenic Shivers: Why We Get the Chills When We Aren’t Cold, Aeon Magazine, 8 Apr. 2021, https://aeon.co/ideas/psychogenic-shivers-why-we-get-the-chills-when-we-arent-cold.

Schoeller, Felix. “Psychogenic Shivers: Why We Get the Chills When We Aren't Cold: Aeon Ideas.” Psychogenic shivers: why we get the chills when we aren’t cold. Aeon Magazine, April 8, 2021. https://aeon.co/ideas/psychogenic-shivers-why-we-get-the-chills-when-we-arent-cold.

Schoeller, F. (2018) Psychogenic shivers: why we get the chills when we aren’t cold. Aeon, 4 June. Available at: https://aeon.co/ideas/psychogenic-shivers-why-we-get-the-chills-when-we-arent-cold.

Schoeller F. Psychogenic shivers: why we get the chills when we aren’t cold [Internet]. Dresser S, editor. 2018. Available from: https://aeon.co/ideas/psychogenic-shivers-why-we-get-the-chills-when-we-arent-cold

 

Text Body

Why We Experience Chills?

A few years ago, I proposed that the feeling of cold in one’s spine, while for example watching a film or listening to music, corresponds to an event when our vital need for cognition is satisfied. Similarly, I have shown that chills are not solely related to music or film but also to the practice of science (mainly physics and mathematics) and to the social logic of religious rituals. I believe that chills and aesthetic emotions in general can teach us something that we do not know yet. They can help us to understand what truly matters to the mind and to the society of minds.

When cold or sick, humans shiver. Shivering is a muscle tremor that produces heat which allows the body to maintain its core temperature in a changing world. Human core temperature can vary temporarily between about 28 to 42 degrees Celsius. Outside these thresholds, death occurs. Humans also shiver in the case of a fever, as heat slows down the rate of pathogen growth and improves the immune response of a living body. Goosebumps or piloerection (the bristling of hairs) can be side effects, as the muscle tremor causes hair to become erect which creates a thin layer of air, thus minimizing heat loss. Strangely enough, humans also shiver independently of any such events. For instance, certain social situations seem to provoke shivers.

Humans are particularly prone to shiver when a group does or thinks the same thing at the same time. When a crowd is sharing a common goal. When they listen to a national anthem or witness self-sacrifice. When they die for their ideas. When collective thought becomes more important than individual life. But humans also shiver from situations that are not social in nature. Some shiver when they manage to find a solution to certain mathematical problems for example, and so shivering cannot be reduced to a social mechanism.

Now, why does a psychological event trigger a physiological response related to the regulation of temperature? At a fundamental level, cognition requires change. If you stabilize a retina using adequate instruments, the organ ceases to transmit signals to the primary visual cortex, and one gradually becomes blind. From the standpoint of the sense organ, the same object never appears similar to itself twice. Two chairs are never exactly the same. In other words, one is constantly discovering a visual field. Everything you feel, you feel for the first time. Perception is really exploration and, if we can perceive anything at all, it is because we are constantly matching incoming sensory signals to available mental models. You rarely fail to recognize objects in your surroundings. The world is always already meaningful, and it is sometimes beautiful.

The process by which a mind adapts to its world is so effective that people constantly mistake one for the other. When a large part of thought matches a large part of the world, one might consciously feel what we call aesthetic emotions. Historically, aesthetics is the science of how perception meets cognition, the science of how you know what you see. The majority of aesthetic emotions are unconscious. They occur every time you see something. When you see something important enough, you might experience these emotions consciously. This happens through bodily changes such as tears, heartbeat increase, sweat – or shivers. The strange thing with shivering is that humans seem to shiver both when they are perfectly capable of predicting the behavior of external objects in real-time when it all fits together so well, and, surprisingly, when nothing at all can be predicted, when the situation goes out of control.

I propose that psychogenic shivers correspond to an event where the measure of the total similarity between all sensory signals and available mental models reaches a local peak value.

I propose that psychogenic shivers correspond to an event where the measure of the total similarity between all sensory signals and available mental models reaches a local peak value. This can be expressed mathematically in terms of the rate of change of a function of conditional similarity. In this context, any change in learning corresponds to an aesthetic emotion. When the function reaches a local maximum, its derivative tends toward zero, and learning slows down. This corresponds to a ‘turning’ point in your total knowledge. Ten years ago, Perlovsky predicted that such an event should involve knowledge about other minds and about the meaning of life.

We know that psychogenic shivers can be inhibited by an excitant, the opioid-antagonist naloxone. Naloxone is what you would inject in a clinical setting to a patient who is victim of an overdose; it is the antagonist of morphine. It does not come as a surprise that most of my subjects state that they relax after they experience an aesthetic shiver. Besides a clear analogy with the sexual drive, what does this tell us about the exploratory drive?

I argue that stories that provoke the shivers might bring about this relief of tension by allowing humans to overcome conflicts among fundamental parts of the mind. Such stories might help us to deal with internal contradictions, where both elements are equally resistant to change. Leon Festinger, who in 1957 invented the theory of cognitive dissonance, named this a dissonance of maximum amplitude. The mind creates stories to overcome its own contradictions. Anthropologists call this a myth, and we know from a wealth of work in anthropology that rituals are likely to provoke shivers down the spine.

We give two examples for such fundamental conflicts; one is biological and the other cultural. The biological conflict derives from the fact that, while we survive as a species by sharing goals, we might never access the goal of other minds directly. We thus shiver in cases of seemingly total communication – theoretical synchrony. Another example derives from the fundamental discordance between the altruistic nature of the human animal on the one hand, and the logic of the currently dominant social system on the other. These hypotheses would explain why you might shiver in the course of a film when empathy becomes a necessary condition to reduce narrative tension to its minimum. When the bad guy ends up saving the good guy.

There are three plausible explanations for the fundamental relation between cognition and temperature. One is physiological, the other is physical, and the third is biological. The physiological explanation simply consists of describing psychogenic shivers as a case of fever. The relation between emotion and temperature is in fact very ancient, and even reptiles display evidence of stress-induced hyperthermia.

The physical explanation relates the dissipation of heat at the shiver to the processing of information in the brain. In 1961 the physicist Rolf Landauer at IBM proposed the principle that any erasure of information should be accompanied by the dissipation of heat. This was verified experimentally a few years ago in Lyon. If this hypothesis is not entirely false, then we should eventually be able to predict the amount of heat produced, given accurate knowledge of the information process. Until then, I do not see any good reason to quantify the shiver.

Finally, the biological explanation relates the origins of human thought to the tremendous changes in temperature at its birth. It might be that we can observe this relation between the mechanisms that regulate cognition and the mechanisms that regulate temperature because of the particular context in which thought saw the light of day. In other words, a shiver might have very well accompanied the first human idea. Since then, every time we grasp something important, perhaps we repeat the gesture.

Read more
neuroaesthetics

Frontiers Research Collection: Possible Applications of Neuroaesthetics To Normal and Pathological Behaviour

E4001, Review

 

Dr. Felix Schoeller is now a Topic Editor for the Frontiers Research Collection under the topic: Possible Applications of Neuroaesthetics To Normal and Pathological Behaviour

 

 

See The Research Collection
Text Body

Frontiers Research Collection: Possible Applications of Neuroaesthetics To Normal and Pathological Behaviour

The aim of this Research Topic is to clarify the role of aesthetic experiences in driving cognitive-emotional change in everyday life at an individual, interpersonal and group level. We are specifically interested in empirical studies of aesthetic emotions in relation to learning and psychopathology. Modern aesthetics — i.e., the science of what is sensed — was originally intended as an alternative to the philosophy of knowledge; in Baumgarten’s words: “the science of sensory knowledge directed toward beauty” (Baumgarten, 1750). In the past few decades, there has been a renewed interest in this relation between aesthetic emotion and knowledge acquisition. Recent evidence suggests that the perception of beauty may subtend the learning process -i.e., the update of perceptual, affective and relational expectations and behavioural plans- and, as such, the somatic markers of aesthetic emotions could serve as potential biomarkers for transient states of enhanced brain plasticity. These findings extend neuroaesthetic research to a wide range of human activities focused on learning and cognitive change, such as education and healthcare.

 

We invite researchers to join forces to document, investigate and understand the role of aesthetic experiences in driving change. We expect contributions deeply rooted in neuro-behavioural data and drawing from multidisciplinary approaches, where experimental and applied aesthetic research may dialogue: neurosciences, clinical and experimental neuropsychology, cognitive science, neurocomputational modelling, experimental psychology, clinical and developmental psychology, psychotherapy and psychiatry, as well as neurorehabilitation.

The key domains of application that will be considered in the special issue are the following:

-Learning/Education. Learning models and research on memory (e.g., how do aesthetic principles potentiate learning), design of timelines and spaces for learning, teaching and educational activities in general.

-Mental Health and psychotherapy: possible application of neuroaesthetic principles to psychotherapy, clinical settings and neurorehabilitation contexts. How do aesthetic competences and aesthetic settings serve diagnostic, rehabilitation and therapeutic processes? Research on the aesthetic variables in the therapeutic encounter: e.g., how do aesthetic sensibility and aesthetic tools/practices influence the therapy of neurological/psychopathological conditions?

-Normal and pathological learning: how do aesthetic emotions influence learning processes and plasticity in normal individuals and in psychiatric and neurological patients? Can we obtain a better understanding of the neurocognitive mechanisms subtending psychopathological behaviour using neuroaesthetic principles?

Read more